It's The Fear Of Death Makes Killing Animals Seem Okay

Reminders of death make people more likely to support killing animals, regardless of their existing attitudes about animal rights, according to new research.

The research provides new insight into the psychology behind humans’ willingness to kill animals for a variety of reasons, and could also potentially help scientists better understand the psychological motivations behind the murder and genocide of humans, says lead researcher Uri Lifshin, a doctoral student in the University of Arizona’s psychology department.

Lifshin and his colleagues conducted a series of experiments based on their existing work on terror management theory—the idea that humans’ awareness of their own mortality is a strong motivator for behaviors that may help quell the fear of death.

“Sometimes, our self-esteem depends on the idea that we are special and not just sacks of meat.”

During the experiments, half of participants were presented with a subliminal or subtle “death prime;” either they saw the word “dead” flash briefly on a computer screen or they saw an image of a T-shirt featuring a skull made up of several iterations of the word “death.”


innerself subscribe graphic


The other half of participants—the controls—instead saw the word “pain” or “fail” flash across the screen, or they saw an image of a plain T-shirt.

Study participants then rated how much they agree with a series of statements about killing animals, such as, “It is often necessary to control for animal overpopulation through different means, such as hunting or euthanasia,” or, “An experiment should never cause the killing of animals.” The researchers avoided asking questions about some of the more broadly accepted justifications for killing animals, like doing so for food.

In all experiments, those who received the death prime were more likely to support killing animals.

Prior to the start of experiments, participants were asked to report their feelings about animal rights. Surprisingly, it didn’t matter if people self-identified as supporters of animal rights. While those individuals were overall less likely than others to support killing animals, the death prime still had the same effect on them.

“If you’re an animal lover or if you care about animals rights, then overall, yes, you are going to support the killing of animals much less; however when you’re reminded of death you’re still going to be a little bit more reactive,” Lifshin says. It’s worth noting that the study did not include overt animal rights activists, who might be affected differently. Additional research is needed for that population, Lifshin says.

Gender also didn’t change the effect of the death prime. Consistent with existing literature, male participants were generally more likely than females to support killing animals, but the death prime affected men and women the same way.

Feeling superior

A paper on the work appears in the Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. The coauthors base their findings on psychology’s terror management theory, which comes from anthropologist Ernest Becker’s 1974 Pulitzer Prize-winning book, The Denial of Death. The theory posits that humans use self-esteem as a buffer against fear of death.

In a previous study, Lifshin and his colleagues showed that when people who enjoy playing basketball are reminded of their mortality, they improve their performance on the basketball court, and thereby their self-esteem, to manage their fear of death.

In the animal study, researchers think death-primed participants supported killing animals more because it provided them with a sense of power or superiority over animals that indirectly helped them fend off fear of mortality, Lifshin says.

This all happens subconsciously.

“Sometimes, our self-esteem depends on the idea that we are special and not just sacks of meat. We want to feel powerful, immortal—not like an animal,” says Lifshin, a proud pet owner whose own love of animals is, in part, what drove him to study why anyone would do them harm.

To further test the terror management connection, Lifshin and his colleagues designed one of their experiments to look at whether giving participants an alternative self-esteem boost would change the effect of the death prime.

It did.

Before each of the experiments conducted by Lifshin and his colleagues, participants were told a cover story to conceal the researchers’ actual aim. In the self-esteem boost experiment, participants heard they were taking part in a word relationship study, and were asked to identify whether pairs of word on a computer screen were related. During the course of the experiment, the word “dead” appeared on the screen for 30 milliseconds to some participants.

When the experimenters praised those who had seen the death prime—telling them: “Oh wow, I’m not sure I’ve seen a score this high on this task, this is really good”—the effect of the death prime was eliminated when participants went on to answer the questions about killing animals. In other words, seeing the death prime did not make participants more supportive of killing animals if they subsequently received a self-esteem boost from a different source.

“We didn’t find that people’s general state of self-esteem made a difference; it was this self-esteem boost,” Lifshin says. “Once your self-esteem is secured, you no longer need to satisfy the need for terror management by killing animals.”

Those who saw the death prime and were given neutral feedback from the experimenters (“OK you did good, just as well as most people do on this task”) still supported killing animals more. The neutral feedback did not change the effect of the death prime.

Dehumanizing people

When researchers asked participants to rate statements about killing humans under various conditions, the death prime did not have the same effect; those who saw the death prime were not more likely to support killing humans.

Even so, the research could still have important implications for the study of the psychology behind murder and genocide of humans who fall into outgroups because of their race, religion, or other characteristics, since those individuals tend to be dehumanized by those who would do them harm, Lifshin says.

“We dehumanize our enemies when there is genocide. There is research in social psychology showing that if you go to places where genocide is happening and you ask the people who are doing the killing to try to explain, they’ll often say things like, ‘Oh, they’re cockroaches, they’re rats, we just have to kill them all,'” Lifshin says. “So if we ever want to really understand how to reduce or fight human-to-human genocide, we have to understand our killing of animals.”

Source: University of Arizona

Related Books

at InnerSelf Market and Amazon